Skip to main content

Dialectical behavior therapy in autistic adults: effects on ecological subjective and physiological measures of emotion dysregulation

Abstract

Background

Although Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) and physiological measurements provide a valuable opportunity to evaluate therapeutic interventions in real time, no study has used this approach to assess Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) in autistic adults with high levels of emotion dysregulation (ED).

Methods

In this study, 26 autistic adults were evaluated before and after participating in a standard 5-month DBT program, using Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA). The EMA included: (1) twelve evaluations per day over a 7-day period, measuring alexithymia, emotional states, subjective arousal and emotion control; (2) continuous physiological monitoring with a wristband to record heart-rate (HR), heart-rate variability (HRV) and skin conductance levels (SCL).

Results

Following DBT, no significant differences were found with respect to negative emotions and higher conflicting emotions, but increased rates of identified emotions, positive emotions and emotion control were found. Baseline autonomic responses remained unchanged, whereas subjective arousal was found to correlate positively with HRV. Overall, these results suggest that participants showed enhanced emotion awareness and emotion regulation capabilities following DBT.

Conclusion

Our study adds to previous research showing that DBT is efficient in treating ED in autistic adults, using real-time measurements of subjective and physiological markers collected through EMA. Specifically, alexithymia measures decreased post-DBT while positive emotions and emotion control increased. Randomized controlled trials should consider using these methods to improve the assessment of the impact of DBT in the daily life of autistic individuals with ED and/or suicidal behavior.

Introduction

Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) is widely acknowledged as the leading intervention for emotion dysregulation (ED) [1,2,3,4,5]. DBT guides individuals to remain present, recognise and accept emotions as they arise, and act in alignment with their values and goals, through the use of mindfulness and emotion regulation strategies [6, 7]. In addition to its efficacy in targeting ED and suicidal behavior in various psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., borderline personality disorder (BPD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)) [5], recent studies have validated the feasibility, acceptability and efficacy of DBT in treating ED in autistic individuals [8,9,10,11,12].

The surge in research examining ED interventions for Autism Spectrum Condition (ASC) can be attributed to the growing acknowledgement of the emotional challenges encountered by this population [13,14,15,16,17]. For instance, the biosocial model for ASC, adapted from that for BPD, posits that ED in autistic adults may also result from an interaction between biological vulnerabilities (i.e., hypersensitivity, hyperreactivity, slow return to baseline and impulsivity) and an invalidating environment (e.g., neglect, abuse) [18,19,20]. Apart from the biosocial correlates shared with individuals with BPD, autistic individuals often display atypical responses to sensory information and are particularly sensitive to changes in their environment [21]. These sensory and adaptive differences can increase emotional reactivity and lability, contributing to distinctive regulatory strategies [22, 23]. For instance, self-soothing behaviors (e.g., rocking, hand-flapping) are common among autistic individuals and may function as protective mechanisms to regulate emotional arousal [24]. However, reliance on other strategies, such as avoidance or rumination, can perpetuate dysregulation [25,26,27]. In some cases, as it is also observed in individuals with BPD, maladaptive behaviors may escalate to include uncontrolled outbursts, aggression [28, 29], and even self-injury and suicidal behaviors [30, 31].

Notably, autistic adults are at a significantly elevated risk of suicidal behaviors [32], with suicidal attempts rates ranging from 7 to 47%, and suicidal ideation occurring in up to 72% of cases [33]. The relationship between suicidal behavior and ED has been linked to the intensity and the physiology of negative emotions [34,35,36,37]. For instance, in people with BPD [34] and adolescents seeking treatment for various psychiatric conditions (e.g., post-traumatic stress disorder, ADHD) [35], research has highlighted that higher levels of negative emotions and lower positive affectivity predicted suicidal risk, with negative emotions decreasing after suicidal behavior [38, 39]. Physiological markers also play a critical role in understanding these dynamics. For example, heart rate variability (HRV), defined as the variation in time intervals between consecutive heartbeats, and heart rate (HR), defined as the number of heart beats per minute, have been observed to change in response to stress and emotional states [40, 41]. Specifically, a reduction in HRV and a slower return to resting HR (i.e., higher HR levels) have been noted in individuals with suicidal ideation and a history of suicide attempts [36, 42].

This complex interplay between negative emotions and suicidal behavior may also be linked to alexithymia, a condition particularly prevalent in ASC [43]. Alexithymia refers to the difficulty in identifying, describing and distinguishing emotions from bodily sensations, as well a tendency to orient thoughts towards external rather than internal stimuli [44]. Alexithymia can hinder effective emotion regulation [45, 46], as it is challenging to control emotions that one cannot clearly identify [19]. Indeed, most studies have reported that, to reduce negative internal states, alexithymic individuals tend to rely mainly on avoidant coping mechanisms, such as experiential avoidance (i.e., avoidance of aversive internal experiences) [47, 48]. Given their increased use of dysfunctional emotion regulation strategies, negative emotions may prove challenging to soothe once they have been aroused [49], explaining the association of alexithymia with the increased risk of suicidal behavior [50]. In the field of physiological research, the evidence at baseline or in response to stressful stimuli suggests that some autistic individuals, especially those presenting with co-occurring ED, are physiologically hyper-aroused [51, 52]. This often includes elevated HR, reduced HRV, and elevated electrical conductance of the skin based on sweat gland activity (i.e., skin conductance levels; SCL) [51]. Chronic hyperarousal has been linked to a reduction in social-reciprocity and the emergence of maladaptive and aggressive behaviors, both towards oneself and others [53, 54]. Yet, whereas some studies have suggested that alexithymia is associated with hyperarousal [55,56,57], others suggest that individuals with high levels of alexithymia show discrepancies between physiological responses and subjective self-reported experiences [58, 59]. This phenomenon is known as the decoupling hypothesis, and its explanations are related to the presence of poorly differentiated emotional responses and difficulties accurately interpreting bodily signals (i.e., impaired interoception). Although such discrepancies have been studied in populations with alexithymia, they remain underexplored in autistic individuals. Considering the high prevalence of alexithymia in ASC [43], it is possible that similar patterns of decoupling may occur in autistic individuals, potentially contributing to ED [60,61,62].

Given the relevance of alexithymia and ED in the context of ASC, interventions such as DBT are of paramount importance, as DBT has the potential to enhance individuals’ awareness of their physiological sensations and internal emotional states [63]. Adaptations of DBT for autistic functioning, such as the use of visual aids to elucidate abstract concepts [8, 10], have fostered accessibility and efficacy within this population. For instance, a recent randomised control trial (RCT) showed that DBT was associated with a reduction in suicidal behaviors and depression in autistic adults in comparison with treatment as usual [9]. However, at the 6-month follow-up, only the reduction in depression remained significant, suggesting that while DBT shows promise, its long-term efficacy warrants further investigation. It is noteworthy that, to the best of our knowledge, this study is the sole RCT published on DBT in ASC. However, it did not evaluate ED or alexithymia. This lacuna is to some extent addressed by a recent systematic review across various clinical groups with ED, including in autistic adults, which indicated that DBT-based interventions may be associated with self-reported improvements in the identification of emotional states [64]. Nevertheless, it is important to highlight that few studies have assessed the afficacy of DBT in ASC, and they have relyied on retrospective measurements. Although informative, such methods may be particularly problematic for individuals with alexithymia, as their difficulty in recognizing and interpreting emotions can lead to biases in retrospective self-reports [65, 66]. Furthermore, this approach lacks ecological validity, as they fail to capture the real-time emotional challenges autistic individuals experience [67, 68].

One potential solution to this issue is the measurement of affective, contextual, and physiological experiences as individuals engage in their usual daily activities [69]. This approach is known as experience sampling, or Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) [67], and has already been employed to assess the impact of DBT among individuals with different psychiatric disorders who engage in non-suicidal self-injury [70,71,72]. Given that previous studies have reported minimal to moderate correspondences between retrospective self-reports and EMA [73], there is a need for more ecological studies to track intervention effects. Nevertheless, to date, no EMA study has evaluated the effect of DBT in ASC. To this purpose, our study aims at gaining a better understanding of the effects of DBT on everyday subjective and physiological functioning of emotions in autistic adults with ED. To this end, over the course of a one-week period prior and after treatment (i.e., pre- and post-18-week standard DBT), 27 autistic individuals responded to 12 notifications per day, to evaluate the nature, the intensity and the control they exerted over their emotions. Concomitantly, physiological parameters such as HR, HRV, and SCL were measured through a wearable device designed for real-time data collection.

Given the established link between DBT and decreased levels alexithymia [64], we hypothesize that it will result in (1a) a reduction of EMA measures of alexithymia (i.e., “I have no emotion” and “I have an emotion I cannot identify”), (1b) a reduction in negative emotions, particularly anxiety and sadness, as well as (1c) fewer conflicting emotions (i.e., the presence of at least two emotions with different valences, such as concomitant “anger” and “joy”), reflective of diminished self-reported psychological distress [74]. Additionally, we hypothesize that, post-DBT, autistic individuals will present (2a) lower physiological sympathetic activity, as measured through SCL, and changes in sympathovagal balance, indicated by variations in HR and HRV and (2b) less pronounced discrepancies between subjective and physiological arousal. Specifically, we expect a more consistent alignment between subjective reports of emotional arousal and physiological measures; for instance, high subjective emotional arousal is frequently observed to be positively correlated with increased HR, although it should be noted that increased HR can also be the result of other factors. Finally, given that DBT strives to help individuals build “a life worth living” [6], we expect that, following DBT, autistic individuals will report (3a) enhanced emotion control, (3b) and higher rates of positive emotions.

Methods

Participants

In the current research, we aimed to explore pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of subjective and physiological EMA-related emotional functioning. Initially, a total of 29 autistic adults participated in DBT from May 2022 to July 2023 and completed EMA assessments before therapy. Three participants dropped out of therapy, resulting in a total of 26 individuals with pre- and post-DBT EMA. Participants received monetary compensation for their participation (€200). The regional ethics committee for the East of France approved this study (Reference: SI 21.01.21.41923). Before participating, all subjects provided written informed consent. This study is part of a larger project, which has been registered in clinicaltrials.gov (Registration Number: NCT04737707). The larger project involved (a) a RCT on the efficacy of DBT in autistic individuals with high ED, self-injury and/or suicidal behaviors, and (b) an EMA study tackling ED in ASC [75]. Specifically, the registered RCT was designed to investigate the efficacy of DBT using retrospective measures, with a waiting list control group and follow-up assessments. In contrast, the present study aims to examine the feasibility and preliminary effects of DBT using EMA as a complement to the findings of the RCT, which included retrospective assessments and targeted measures of self-harm and suicidal behaviors. Due to the constraints of feasibility, the EMA approach was unable to include a waiting list group our follow-up assessments, and prioritised real-time assessments of emotional functioning (e.g., emotion control and alexithymia). Therefore, these results are presented as preliminary. For the RCT, the main outcome was the DERS total score. Hence, the minimal sample size was estimated based on assumptions on distribution of DERS scores reported in Bemmouna et al.’s pilot study [8]. Assumptions included a margin of error set at 0.05 and a minimal statistical power of 90%. A sample of at least 48 participants was required to demonstrate a reduction of at least 10 points in the DERS mean score at mid-therapy (T1) compared to baseline (T0) with a power of 95%. Additionally, the study aimed to demonstrate a reduction of at least 15 points at post-therapy (T2) and six-month follow-up (T3) compared to baseline, with a statistical power of 91%. To account for potential dropouts, 16 additional patients were included, representing approximately one-third of the required sample.

Participants (15 female and 11 male) had an average age of 28.5 (SD = 10.3, range = 18–67). The diagnosis of ASC without intellectual disabilities was established through the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [76] and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 [77], with all participants undergoing a comprehensive DSM-5 evaluation for co-occurring psychiatric disorders [21]. This assessment revealed that 85% (n = 22) had a co-occurring condition, with the most prevalent being depression (50%), anxiety disorders (45%), and ADHD (36%) – see Table 1. Given the presence of co-occurrent disorders, psychotropic medication was observed in 88% of autistic individuals. Because some medication have significant anticholinergic effects which can influence physiological parameters [78], we employed an anticholinergic scale – The Anticholinergic Impregnation Scale [79] - to account for their effects in our statistical analysis. The scale ranged from 0 (no medication or medication with low anticholinergic effect) to 5 (multiple medications with very strong anticholinergic effect). Furthermore, all participants completed The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale - DERS [80] and the Eight-item General Alexithymia Factor Score – GAFS-8 [81] at two time points: before (pre-) and after (post-) DBT. The baseline (pre-) scores were employed to describe the sample, which was selected on the basis of elevated levels of ED, while the post- scores were analysed to assess changes in ED and alexithymia following treatment.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of autistic adults

DBT intervention

Participants engaged in a standard DBT program for 5 months. Standard DBT comprises the four modes of treatment developed by Linehan [6]: (i) weekly individual sessions, (ii) weekly 2-hours and 15-minute skills training groups, (iii) phone coaching, and (iv) consultation team. The therapy was delivered by three clinical psychologists extensively trained in DBT. Weekly supervision sessions were provided by LW, professor of clinical psychology. In order to adapt DBT to the needs of autistic adults, several modifications were implemented. These included maintaining a stable therapy environment, addressing sensory sensitivities, simplifying materials, and incorporating visual tools (e.g., an emotional thermometer). Further details on these adaptations are discussed by Bemmouna et al. [8] ([76], p. 4340].

Self-reported EMA surveys

To evaluate the subjective self-reported effects of DBT, all participants responded to real-time mobile phone notifications delivered 12 times per day, for a period of 7 days, both before and after the therapy. Using Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Provo, UT) and the Textra app on a Samsung Galaxy XCover 5 device, the notifications were sent between 7 am and 11 pm, tailored to each participant’s chronotype. The average interval between each survey was 65 min, ranging from 43 to 84 min. Participants were also encouraged to complete the assessment if they were experiencing an intense emotion outside of scheduled notifications.

At each EMA survey, participants were required to respond to six questions, as described in [75]. Firstly, they selected one or more options from a list of 12 emotional categories to characterise their current emotional state: joy, anxiety, anger, interest, shame, disgust, calm, sadness, surprise, guilt, I have an emotion that I cannot name, and I have no emotion. Participants had the possibility to choose multiple emotions at a time, as previous EMA studies have shown that, compared to control individuals, participants with ED overreport conflicting emotions (i.e., the presence of at least two emotions with different valences, such as concomitant “anger” and “joy”) [74, 75]. Secondly, participants rated the intensity of their selected emotions (i.e., subjective arousal) on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (i.e., no intensity) to 10 (i.e., high intensity). Thirdly, they evaluated their perceived level of control over emotions (i.e., emotion control) on a similar single-item scale, from 0 to 10, with higher scores indicating greater perceived control. Next, participants indicated their preference for emotion regulation by selecting one of three options: (a) ‘I would prefer to maintain my current emotional state’; (b) ‘I would prefer to increase my current emotional state’; and (c) ‘I would prefer to decrease my current emotional state’. Furthermore, they evaluated their level of fatigue on a scale from 0 (i.e., no fatigue) to 10 (i.e., severe fatigue). Finally, to assess contextual factors, participants reported on their physical activity and social interactions during the 15 min preceding the survey. For physical activity, they indicated whether they had engaged in (a) no activity; (b) light activity; or (c) intense physical activity. For social interactions, they selected from six predefined categories: (a) ‘I had no interactions; I was alone in a closed environment’; (b) ‘I had no interactions; I was alone in an open environment’; (c) ‘I had no interactions; other people were present in the same environment’; (d) ‘I had a direct social interaction’; (e) ‘I had a virtual social interaction (e.g., an online meeting)’; and (f) ‘I had an indirect social interaction (e.g., text messages)’.

Physiological parameter monitoring

To measure real-time physiological responses in relation to emotions, participants wore the Empatica E4 wristband for 7 days, alongside subjective EMA pre- and post-DBT participation [82]. The device recorded SC and PPG signals for 12 h each day. In light of the EMA question concerning physical activity, which specified a 15-minute timeframe, we analyzed the segments of physiological signals recorded during the 15 min preceding each questionnaire. These segments were selected based on two criteria: firstly, the availability of the data, which was dependent on whether the subject was wearing the device during the relevant period; and secondly, the signal quality. Any segments affected by motion artifacts were excluded.

The SC signal is modulated by the sweat gland activity, which is controlled by the sympathetic nervous system activity. The SC signal can be divided into a tonic component, known as skin conductance level (SCL), which indicates overall arousal state and varies slowly, and a superimposed phasic component, known as skin conductance response (SCR), which reflects faster stimulus-related reactions. In this study, we focused on the SCL to monitor the overall state of the subject, as no specific protocol was administered. The SCL was obtained by applying a low-pass filter with a cutoff frequency of 0.05 Hz, to isolate the tonic component of the skin conductance. Hereinafter, we define SCL as the mean value of the tonic component, averaged within non-overlapping 30-second time windows across the entire segments of analyzed data.

The Empatica provides the average heat rate values computed in spans of 10 s, derived from the blood volume pulse (BVP). We computed the average within non-overlapping 5-minute time windows across the segments of analyzed data to obtain the heart rate (HR) mean. Finally, from the BVP, the Empatica provides also the interbeat interval (IBI) series. Therefore, we extracted the HRV as the standard deviation of the IBI within non-overlapping 5-minute time windows across the selected segments of data. Participants were instructed to upload the data via the Empatica Manager App at the end of each day. This allowed the processing of the data and the extraction of the SCL, HR and HRV.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using Jamovi [83]. To assess changes in self-reported emotions (Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c and 3b) before and after therapy, we conducted paired samples t-tests for normally distributed data (Shapiro-Wilk test; p >.05) and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (Shapiro-Wilk test, p <.05) for nonparametric data. Cohen’ s d effect sizes and rank biserial correlations were calculated for all comparisons. As for Hypotheses 1a, 1b and 3b, based on the normality test, we conducted either paired samples t-tests or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests and reported effect sizes.

Differences in emotion control, physiological arousal, and discordances between physiological and subjective arousal were analysed using Multilevel Model Analysis (MLM), a suitable tool for analysing repeated measures [84]. In this study, we employed a restricted maximum likelihood estimation method [85], and the hierarchical structure of MLM allowed us to explore variations in outcome variables (i.e., emotion control, HR, HRV and SCL) across different levels: observations within a day, days within a person, and among different persons. The final MLM aimed to predict the outcome variables as described in Eq. 1. Specifically, we accounted for variability in intercepts (random intercepts) for the 12 EMA evaluations per day (1/Q), the 26 participants (1/ID), and the seven days (1/Day). To control for variables potentially influencing subjective emotion control and physiological responses, we included age, sex, medication, the presence of anxiety disorders, and EMA of social interactions and physical activity in the 15 min prior to each evaluation. To test hypothesis 2b (diminished discrepancy between physiological and subjective emotional arousal post-therapy), and hypothesis 3a (post-DBT enhanced emotion control, independently of emotional intensity), we added the variable “Subjective Arousal” and its interaction with time (Subjective Arousal: Time) to the models. Slopes for EMA variables varied across the 12 evaluations (1 + Social Interaction + Physical Activity + Subjective Arousal / Q), while fixed slopes were used for demographic data and the pre-post DBT condition (Time). To facilitate interpretation and comparison of predictors, and to reduce multicollinearity in the analysis, all continuous variables (i.e., Emotion Control, HR, HRV, SCL, Age, Subjective Arousal) were standardised using z-scores (i.e., each value was transformed to represent the number of standard deviations from the variable’s mean). Categorical variables were not centered [86].

Equation 1

  1. a).

    Hypothesis 2b

$$\begin{aligned} \:Physiological&\: Variable\:\left(HR;\:HRV;\:SCL\right)\sim\:1+\:Age\:+\:Sex\:\\&+\:Medication\:+\:Anxiety\:Disorders\:\\&+\:Social\:Interaction\:+\:Physical\:Activity\:\\&+\:Time\:+\:Subjective\:Arousal\:\\&+\:Subjective\:Arousal\::Time\:+\left(1\right|Q)+\:(1\left|ID\right)\\&+\left(1\right|Day)+\:(1+\:Social\:Interaction\:\\&+\:Physical\:Activity\:+Subjective\:Arousal\:\left|\:Q\right)\end{aligned}$$
  1. b).

    Hypothesis 3a

$$\begin{aligned} \:Emotion\:Control&\:\sim\:1+\:Age\:+\:Sex\:+\:Medication\:\\&+\:Anxiety\:Disorders\:+\:Social\:Interaction\:\\&+\:Physical\:Activity\:+\:Time\:\\&+\:Subjective\:Arousal\:\\&+\:Subjective\:Arousal\::Time\:+\left(1\right|Q)\\&+\:(1\left|ID\right)+\left(1\right|Day)+\:(1+\:Social\:Interaction\:\\&+\:Physical\:Activity\:+Subjective\:Arousal\:\left|\:Q\right)\end{aligned}$$

Results

EMA surveys description

Participants completed a total of 4.072 EMA evaluations across the pre-post DBT phases (EMApre=2046; EMApost=2026). There were no significant differences in the number of responses between the two phases (p = .757). Specifically, each participant completed an average of 78.7 EMA (SD = 9.23, range: 44–89) before DBT, and 77.9 EMA (SD = 8.62; range: 46–87) after DBT. The compliance rate, defined as the percentage of answered prompts relative to the total number of prompts sent, was 92.3% (SD = 10.7; range: 50–100) before DBT and 92.2% (SD = 10.5; range: 51.2–100) after DBT.

Pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of emotional frequencies: hypotheses 1a, 1b, and 3b

Descriptive statistics and paired sample comparisons are detailed in Table 2. In line with our first hypothesis (1a), paired samples t-tests and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests revealed that the rates of “I have an emotion I cannot name” were three times lower post-therapy compared to pre-therapy [preDBT= 9.3%, M(SD) = 7.35 (10.54); postDBT= 3.5%, M(SD) = 2.77 (3.8); p = .015)]. However, contrary to our expectations, no significant differences were found between the pre- and post-therapy conditions for the option, “I have no emotion” (p = .165), nor for other negative emotions (e.g., sadness, anxiety; Hypothesis 1b) – see Table 2. When testing our hypothesis regarding the effects of DBT on the presence of positive emotions (3b), results revealed that, post-intervention, autistic individuals reported higher rates of joy [preDBT= 12.5%, M(SD) = 9.85 (7.32); postDBT= 18.7%, M(SD) = 14.62 (11.48); p = .008], calm [preDBT= 25.5%, M(SD) = 20.1 (19.9); postDBT= 34.2%, M(SD) = 26.7 (22.3); p = .026], and interest [preDBT= 8.3%, M(SD) = 6.5 (8.48); postDBT= 15.8%, M(SD) = 12.3 (16.51); p = .025].

Table 2 Descriptive statistics,paired samples t-test and Wilcoxon signed-rank test results for emotion selections and need for emotion regulation

Pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of multiple - conflicting emotions, and need for emotion regulation: hypothesis 1c

Analyses revealed that compared to the preDBT phase, postDBT, autistic individuals had higher rates of multiple emotions (i.e., more than one emotion at a time) [preDBT=22.4%, M(SD) = 17.7(15.8); postDBT=32.4%, M(SD) 25.3 (20.1); p = .020]. Moreover, among these multiple concomitant emotional states, conflicting emotions represented 28.3% of choices in preDBT [M(SD) = 5(5.29)] and 41.1% in postDBT [M(SD) = 10.38 (12.67)], with significant increase between the two conditions (p = .004). With regards to the self-reported need for emotion regulation, no statistical differences were found in the pre- and post-DBT comparisons [need to decrease (p = .162); need to maintain (p = .476); need to increase (p = .082)] (Table 2).

Pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of emotion control: hypothesis 3a

When evaluating the effects of therapy on emotion control, MLM analyses revealed that time (postDBT) was a significant positive predictor [post-hoc difference (SE) = 0.70 (0.05); p <.001] –see Table 3. This effect persisted despite high levels of subjective arousal, which were negatively associated with emotion control [b (SE) =−0.08 (0.01); p <.001] in both conditions (i.e., the interaction between time and arousal revealed no significant association; p = .389) (Fig. 1). Among control predictors, only intense physical activity (compared to no physical activity) was associated with increased levels of emotion control [post-hoc difference (SE) = 0.19 (0.01); p = .035].

Table 3 PostDBT effects on emotion control, HR, HRV, and SCL
Fig. 1
figure 1

Participant-level emotional changes pre- and post- DBT

Pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of physiological manifestations: hypotheses 2a and 2b

Contrary to our hypothesis 2a, no significant differences were found between the preDBT and postDBT conditions in HR (p = .339), HRV (p = .305), and SCL (p = .756) (Table 3). When evaluating hypothesis 2b, results showed that subjective arousal was positively associated with HRV [b(SE) = 0.06 (0.02); p = .003]. However, it was not a significant predictor of HR (p = .485), SCL (p = .103), and no interaction with time was found (preDBT versus postDBT). Among all variables included in the models, the only positive predictor of the three physiological measures was intense physical activity (p <.001). The presence of social interactions was positively associated with HR [b(SE) = 1.40 (0.16); p <.001], and HRV [b(SE) = 0.22 (0.03); p <.001], but not with SCL (p = .143). Moreover, for HR, age and medication intake were also significant predictors, with older age being associated with decreased HR [b(SE) = − 0.13 (0.06); p = .044] and medication intake with increased HR levels [b(SE) = 0.21 (0.06); p = .002].

Pre- and post-DBT differences in terms of retrospective measures of ED and alexithymia

In addition to the prospective subjective and physiological measures, pre- and post- DBT comparisons of retrospective measures were conducted to evaluate changes in ED and alexithymia (see Table 4). Paired sample t-tests demonstrated statistically significant reductions in the total DERS scores (p <.001) and in all six DERS subscales, including Non-Acceptance (p <.001), Goals (p <.001), Impulse (p <.001), Awareness (p <.001)], Strategies (p <.001)], and Clarity (p <.001)]. Additionally, significant reductions were observed in the GAFS-8 scores (p <.001)].

Table 4 Paired sample t-Test for retrospective measures of emotion dysregulation and alexithymia

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first pilot study to explore EMA of subjective and physiological emotional functioning, both pre- and post-DBT, in autistic adults presenting with ED, self-injury and/or suicidal behavior. While preliminary, our findings suggest that, following their participation in a five-month standard DBT, autistic individuals reported a reduction in difficulties associated with identifying emotions (i.e., decreased frequencies of “I have an emotion I cannot name”). Additionally, they reported an increase in positive emotions, including joy, calm, and interest, as well as an enhanced feeling of emotion control in their daily lives. No significant differences were identified in the baseline physiological parameters (i.e., HR, HRV, and SCL), or with respect to negative emotions between the pre- and post-DBT. Contrary to our predictions, an increased prevalence of conflicting emotions was found post-DBT, probably due to reduced alexithymia. Nevertheless, despite the absence of an increase in baseline autonomic resilience following DBT, subjective arousal was found to predict higher HRV. This result may indicate that the participants’ physiological systems showed enhanced capacity to adapt to intense emotional states, as evidenced by changes in HRV.

Contrary to our expectations, rates of multiple emotions, including conflicting (i.e., simultaneous positive and negative) ones, were significantly higher post-therapy. We argue that this unexpected finding may be attributed to the fact that participants reported fewer unidentified emotions (i.e., “I have an emotion I cannot name”), which likely led to the identification of a broader and more complex range of emotions, particularly an increase in the frequency positive emotions. Importantly, this increase in emotion labelling post-DBT is one of the primary outcomes of our study. This improvement aligns with the goals of DBT, which specifically teaches individuals to observe and identify their current emotions, thoughts and physical sensations, through mindfulness and emotion regulation skills [87]. Furthermore, since the potential risks associated with heightened emotional awareness are addressed by DBT modules of distress tolerance (e.g., using self-soothing techniques), it can be argued that the increase in conflicting emotions reflects greater emotional diversity and the emergence of positive emotions rather than a purely maladaptive response. Nevertheless, we recommend future studies further explore this relationship to better understand its implications.

Notably, no significant differences were found in the measure of “I have no emotion” between the pre- and post-therapy assessments. This indicates that, although participants showed an improved ability to identify specific emotions, the overall experience of lacking emotions remained unchanged following DBT. These results are consistent with prior research indicating that DBT effectively addresses certain aspects of alexithymia, such as the ability to identify emotions [64, 70]. The findings presented here contribute to the ongoing discussion regarding the potential of DBT to enhance emotion identification, while also suggesting that the experience of having no emotions, which has been linked to alexithymia in past studies [62, 88], might remain unchanged following DBT in autistic people. Rather than being related to the identification of emotions per se, the latter may be associated with the dimensions of alexithymia that pertain to the differentiation between emotions and physical sensations, as well as the presence of externally oriented thoughts [44, 89].

In addition to the decreased rates of alexithymia-related EMA responses, our study revealed a significant increase post-DBT in EMA responses relative to emotion control and positive emotions. This is consistent with the primary aim of DBT, i.e., to build a life worth living by focusing on ED [6]. Indeed, participants reported elevated rates of positive emotions, including joy, calm, and interest, post-therapy. This outcome is consistent with prior research demonstrating DBT’s efficacy in enhancing quality of life [5] and, in certain instances, in reducing depressive symptoms, anger, aggressive and self-destructive behaviors [4, 9, 90]. However, substantial effects on anger, depression and suicidal ideation are typically observed following extended DBT programmes (i.e., lasting from 4 to 14 months) [91]. It is therefore possible that our 5-month DBT programme may have been too short to significantly reduce the experience of negative emotions. Interestingly, our results suggested that, although the rates of negative emotions remained unchanged over time, participants reported increased emotion control following DBT. This indicates that, irrespective of their valence, the ability to control emotions matters more than their mere presence. This finding is consistent with the therapy’s focus on enhancing emotion experience and emotion regulation, rather than reducing negative or conflicting emotions. Interestingly, enhanced emotion regulation may allow individuals to experience negative and conflicting emotions without increased distress, emphasizing the importance of emotion control in psychological well-being [92]. Overall, our EMA results are therefore congruent with recent research showing reduced suicidal behavior linked to intense self-reported ED following DBT in autistic adults [9], and our own results showing a decrease of ED, measured by the DERS, post-therapy. Importantly, while DBT’s effects on the reduction of suicidal behaviors are well-documented [91], evidence of its effects on ED is less robust [93]. This discrepancy may be attributable to the use of self-report retrospective questionnaires of ED, which are based on various models of ED and are liable to memory biases and alexithymia [65]. The use of EMA methodology may therefore circumvent these problems and offer a more accurate reflection of emotional dynamics and emotion regulation in everyday life [66, 94]. Moreover, the combined use of subjective EMA responses and the measurement of physiological parameters might improve the understanding of mechanisms of change of DBT [66].

While DBT might have been effective in enhancing positive emotions and emotion control, these changes did not directly translate into changes in physiological measures. One possible explanation is related to the absence of significant changes in negative emotions, e.g., anxiety and anger, as negative emotions are closely linked to SCL increase [95]. However, a positive correlation was found between subjective arousal and HRV. Given that HRV is a vagal index commonly associated with relaxation, emotion regulation and autonomic flexibility [96], it is generally linked to resting [97], thereby reduced subjective emotional arousal. One potential explanation relative to this unexpected finding is that the result reflects an increased emotional awareness and an enhanced capacity to adapt to intense emotional states. Consistently, in a prior study, it was demonstrated that elevated resting HRV was predictive of skills in facial emotional expression [98], suggesting that individuals experiencing higher subjective arousal may engage in more effective regulatory strategies (e.g., skills such as distraction, self-soothing, and radical acceptance).

Despite these encouraging findings, the absence of a significant interaction between time (postDBT versus preDBT) and subjective arousal in predicting HR, HRV, or SCL suggests that DBT may not have had a clear impact on physiological responses. This prompts the question of whether the observed relationship between HRV and emotional arousal is stable over time, and whether DBT interventions exert an influence on this relationship. One potential explanation may be related to the physiological measurement employed in this study. It is possible that the use of the wristband Empatica E4 to assess HRV and SCL may have limited sensitivity in terms of detecting subtle within-person changes over time. This is in comparison to more established methods of assessment such as electrocardiogram (i.e., ECG) or laboratory-based equipment used for measuring skin conductance. Although such devices are beneficial for obtaining data in real-world settings [82, 99], they may be more appropriate for identifying significant group-level differences than subtle individual variations.

Our research has some limitations. Firstly, the absence of a control group limits the ability to attribute observed changes specifically to DBT. However, given that similar results have been reported in populations other than ASC using self-reported questionnaires of ED in RCT designs [1, 100], we speculate that the emotion-related changes observed here are likely due to DBT rather than the mere passage of time or the effects of repeated measures. In addition, no prior study has explored DBT’s effects using EMA, underscoring the innovative approach of our research. Secondly, the use of EMA items such as “I have no emotion” and “I have an emotion I cannot name” as measures of alexithymia and “emotion control” as a measure of ED may lack construct validity, as alexithymia and ED are complex constructs that might be only partially captured by these items. Precisely, we acknowledge the limitation of not being able to compute reliability coefficients (e.g., McDonald’s omega) for single-item measures such as those pertaining to emotion control. Nevertheless, this single-item approach permitted precise and concise assessments throughout the intensive EMA protocol, thereby reducing participants’ burden. Thirdly, although contextual information, e.g., physical activity and social interactions, was collected in this study, including more detailed behavioral variables, such as the use of emotion regulation strategies in real-life, might improve our understanding of the impact of DBT on physiological responses. Fourthly, psychophysiological measures were collected using Empatica E4 which may have had limited sensitivity when compared with gold-standard techniques such as ECG. This limitation may have contributed to the absence of significant physiological changes observed in the study. Furthermore, despite controlling for anticholinergic effects, the presence of psychotropic treatment in the sample introduces possible confounding variables. Additionally, the relatively small sample size of 26 participants limits the generalisability of the findings and the statistical power to detect significant effects. This study should therefore be considered preliminary, with results aimed at exploring the feasibility of employing EMA to examine therapy effects of DBT in autistic adults. Finally, the DSM-5 severity level ratings were not considered in our sample, and ASC-severity is likely to be heterogeneous. Yet, recent studies suggest that the DSM-5 severity specifier is influenced by factors unrelated to the ASC-severity (e.g., age, IQ), which limits its clinical utility [101]. Future studies should address these limitations by including control groups, using validated EMA measures of alexithymia and emotion control, gathering more detailed contextual and behavioral data, considering medication effects more comprehensively and including larger samples.

Conclusions

In conclusion, our EMA study contributes to the growing body of evidence supporting the adaptation and application of DBT for autistic adults. Specifically, our findings indicate that DBT shows promise in enhancing certain aspects of emotion regulation in this population, such as increased emotional awareness, as well as enhanced everyday experience of positive emotions and emotion control. While these improvements were not linked to significant changes in physiological measures, it is essential to shift our focus towards the understanding of the real-life mechanisms that foster changes in ED post-DBT. To this aim, the combined use of EMA and real-time physiological monitoring might complement the use of traditional self-report questionnaires and provide valuable insight into the mechanisms of change of DBT.

Data availability

https://osf.io/hfpka/?view_only=9692ee9b65414b1f8a990a9afb213243.

Abbreviations

ADHD:

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

BPD:

Borderline personality disorder

BVP:

Blood Volume Pulse

ECG:

Electrocardiogram

ED:

Emotion dysregulation

EMA:

Ecological momentary assessment

DBT:

Dialectical behavior therapy

HR:

Heart rate

HRV:

Heart-rate variability

MLM:

Multilevel model analysis

NA:

Negative affect

PA:

Positive affect

PPG:

Photoplethismographic signal

RCT:

Randomised control trial

SC:

Skin conductance

SCL:

Skin conductance levels

References

  1. Rozakou-Soumalia N, Dârvariu Ş, Sjögren JM. Dialectical behaviour therapy improves emotion dysregulation mainly in binge eating disorder and bulimia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JPM. 2021;11(9):931.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Ritschel LA, Lim NE, Stewart LM. Transdiagnostic applications of DBT for adolescents and adults. Am J Psychother. 2015;69(2):111–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lungu A, Linehan MM. Dialectical behavior therapy: a comprehensive multi-and transdiagnostic intervention. In: Nezu CM, Nezu AM, editors. The Oxford handbook of cognitive and behavioral therapies. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2016. p. 200–14.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Kothgassner OD, Goreis A, Robinson K, Huscsava MM, Schmahl C, Plener PL. Efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy for adolescent self-harm and suicidal ideation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2021;51(7):1057–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Panos PT, Jackson JW, Hasan O, Panos A. Meta-analysis and systematic review assessing the efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy (DBT). Res Soc Work Pract. 2014;24(2):213–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Linehan MM. Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Robins CJ, Chapman AL. Dialectical behavior therapy: current status, recent developments, and future directions. J Pers Disord. 2004;18(1):73–89.

  8. Bemmouna D, Coutelle R, Weibel S, Weiner L. Feasibility, acceptability and preliminary efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy for autistic adults without intellectual disability: a mixed methods study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2022;52(10):4337–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Huntjens A, Van Den Bosch LMC (Wies), Sizoo B, Kerkhof A, Smit F, Van Der Gaag M, editors. The effectiveness and safety of dialectical behavior therapy for suicidal ideation and behavior in autistic adults: a pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med. 2024;54(10):2707–18. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0033291724000825

  10. Ritschel LA, Guy L, Maddox BB. A pilot study of dialectical behaviour therapy skills training for autistic adults. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2022;50(2):187–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Keenan EG, Gurba AN, Mahaffey B, Kappenberg CF, Lerner MD. Leveling up dialectical behavior therapy for autistic individuals with emotion dysregulation: clinical and personal insights. Autism Adulthood. 2024;6(1):1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Lee H. The preliminary feasibility and efficacy of a modified dialectical behavior therapy skills training on emotion regulation for young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Michigan State University; 2021. https://d.lib.msu.edu/etd

  13. Keluskar J, Reicher D, Gorecki A, Mazefsky C, Crowell JA. Understanding, assessing, and intervening with emotion dysregulation in autism spectrum disorder: a developmental perspective. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2021;30(2):335–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Mazefsky CA. Emotion regulation and emotional distress in autism spectrum disorder: foundations and considerations for future research. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(11):3405–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Samson AC, Phillips JM, Parker KJ, Shah S, Gross JJ, Hardan AY. Emotion dysregulation and the core features of autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(7):1766–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Conner CM, Golt J, Shaffer R, Righi G, Siegel M, Mazefsky CA. Emotion dysregulation is substantially elevated in autism compared to the general population: impact on psychiatric services. Autism Res. 2021;14(1):169–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dell’Osso L, Massoni L, Battaglini S, De Felice C, Nardi B, Amatori G, et al. Emotional dysregulation as a part of the autism spectrum continuum: a literature review from late childhood to adulthood. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:1234518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Bemmouna D, Lagzouli A, Weiner L. The biosocial correlates and predictors of emotion dysregulation in autistic adults compared to borderline personality disorder and nonclinical controls. Mol Autism. 2023;14(1):47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Bemmouna D, Weiner L. Linehan’s biosocial model applied to emotion dysregulation in autism: a narrative review of the literature and an illustrative case conceptualization. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:1238116.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Linehan MM. A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: elaborating and extending Linehan’s theory. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(3):495–510.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Philadelphia: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  22. Brindle K, Moulding R, Bakker K, Nedeljkovic M. Is the relationship between sensory-processing sensitivity and negative affect mediated by emotional regulation? Aust J Psychol. 2015;67(4):214–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Sáez-Suanes GP, García-Villamisar D, Del Pozo Armentia A, Dattilo J. Emotional dysregulation and uncertainty intolerance as transdiagnostic mediators of anxiety in adults with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2020;106:103784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Manor-Binyamini I, Schreiber-Divon M. Repetitive behaviors: listening to the voice of people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disorders. 2019;64:23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Cai RY, Richdale AL, Uljarevic M, Dissanayake C, Samson AC. Emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder: where we are and where we need to go. Autism Res. 2018;11(7):962–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Thompson EL, Gillespie Smith K, Mair APA, Obsuth I. Exploring Emotional Dysregulation and Avoidance with Caregivers as the Mechanisms Linking Social Communication Understanding and Aggressive Behaviours. J Autism Dev Disord. 2024. Available from: https://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-024-06276-8. [cited 2025 Jan 14].

  27. Oliva A, Ferracini SM, Amoia R, Giardinieri G, Moltrasio C, Brambilla P, et al. The association between anger rumination and emotional dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: a review. J Affect Disord. 2023;338:546–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Samson AC, Hardan AY, Lee IA, Phillips JM, Gross JJ. Maladaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorder: the role of emotion experience and emotion regulation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(11):3424–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Sung YS, Lin CY, Chu SY, Lin LY. Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between sensory processing and behavior problems in young children with autism spectrum disorder: a preliminary study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2024;54(2):738–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Maddox BB, Trubanova A, White SW. Untended wounds: non-suicidal self-injury in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2017;21(4):412–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Moseley RL, Gregory NJ, Smith P, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. A ‘choice’, an ‘addiction’, a way ‘out of the lost’: exploring self-injury in autistic people without intellectual disability. Mol Autism. 2019;10(1):18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. OHalloran L, Coey P, Wilson C. Suicidality in autistic youth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2022;93:102144.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Zahid S, Upthegrove R. Suicidality in autistic spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Crisis. 2017;38(4):237–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Rizvi SL, Fitzpatrick S. Changes in suicide and non-suicidal self‐injury ideation and the moderating role of specific emotions over the course of dialectical behavior therapy. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2021;51(3):429–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rojas SM, Leen-Feldner EW, Blumenthal H, Lewis SF, Feldner MT. Risk for suicide among treatment seeking adolescents: the role of positive and negative affect intensity. Cogn Ther Res. 2015;39(2):100–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Neacsiu AD, Fang CM, Rodriguez M, Rosenthal MZ. Suicidal behavior and problems with emotion regulation. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2018;48(1):52–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Sarchiapone M, Gramaglia C, Iosue M, Carli V, Mandelli L, Serretti A, et al. The association between electrodermal activity (EDA), depression and suicidal behaviour: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18(1):22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Kuehn KS, Dora J, Harned MS, Foster KT, Song F, Smith MR, et al. A meta-analysis on the affect regulation function of real-time self-injurious thoughts and behaviours. Nat Hum Behav. 2022;6(7):964–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Andrewes HE, Hulbert C, Cotton SM, Betts J, Chanen AM. An ecological momentary assessment investigation of complex and conflicting emotions in youth with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2017;252:102–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH, Koo BH. Stress and heart rate variability: a meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15(3):235–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Seddon JA, Rodriguez VJ, Provencher Y, Raftery-Helmer J, Hersh J, Labelle PR, et al. Meta-analysis of the effectiveness of the Trier social stress test in eliciting physiological stress responses in children and adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;116:104582.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Wilson ST, Chesin M, Fertuck E, Keilp J, Brodsky B, Mann JJ, et al. Heart rate variability and suicidal behavior. Psychiatry Res. 2016;240:241–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kinnaird E, Stewart C, Tchanturia K. Investigating alexithymia in autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Psychiatr. 2019;55:80–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of alexithymic characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22(2–6):255–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Preece DA, Mehta A, Petrova K, Sikka P, Bjureberg J, Becerra R, et al. Alexithymia and emotion regulation. J Affect Disord. 2023;324:232–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Garofalo C, Velotti P, Zavattini GC. Emotion regulation and aggression: the incremental contribution of alexithymia, impulsivity, and emotion dysregulation facets. Psychol Violence. 2018;8(4):470–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Panayiotou G, Leonidou C, Constantinou E, Hart J, Rinehart KL, Sy JT, et al. Do alexithymic individuals avoid their feelings? Experiential avoidance mediates the association between alexithymia, psychosomatic, and depressive symptoms in a community and a clinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2015;56:206–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Orsolini L. Unable to describe my feelings and emotions without an addiction: the interdependency between alexithymia and addictions. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:543346.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Morie KP, Crowley MJ, Mayes LC, Potenza MN. The process of emotion identification: considerations for psychiatric disorders. J Psychiatr Res. 2022;148:264–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Hemming L, Taylor P, Haddock G, Shaw J, Pratt D. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between alexithymia and suicide ideation and behaviour. J Affect Disord. 2019;254:34–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Arora I, Bellato A, Ropar D, Hollis C, Groom MJ. Is autonomic function during resting-state atypical in autism: a systematic review of evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;125:417–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bellato A, Sesso G, Milone A, Masi G, Cortese S. Systematic review and Meta-Analysis: altered autonomic functioning in youths with emotional dysregulation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2024;63(2):216–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Morabito DM, Boffa JW, Bedford CE, Chen JP, Schmidt NB. Hyperarousal symptoms and perceived burdensomeness interact to predict suicidal ideation among trauma-exposed individuals. J Psychiatr Res. 2020;130:218–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Lorber MF. Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(4):531–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Nandrino JL, Berna G, Hot P, Dodin V, Latrée J, Decharles S, et al. Cognitive and physiological dissociations in response to emotional pictures in patients with anorexia. J Psychosom Res. 2012;72(1):58–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. De Timary P, Roy E, Luminet O, Fillée C, Mikolajczak M. Relationship between alexithymia, alexithymia factors and salivary cortisol in men exposed to a social stress test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(8):1160–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Luminet O, Rimé B, Bagby RM, Taylor G. A multimodal investigation of emotional responding in alexithymia. Cogn Emot. 2004;18(6):741–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Eastabrook JM, Lanteigne DM, Hollenstein T. Decoupling between physiological, self-reported, and expressed emotional responses in alexithymia. Pers Individ Differ. 2013;55(8):978–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Panayiotou G, Panteli M, Vlemincx E. Processing emotions in alexithymia: a systematic review of physiological markers. In: Luminet O, Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, editors. Alexithymia: Advances in research, theory, and clinical practice. 1st ed. Cambridge University Press; 2018. p. 291–320.

  60. Ben Shalom D, Mostofsky SH, Hazlett RL, Goldberg MC, Landa RJ, Faran Y, et al. Normal physiological emotions but differences in expression of conscious feelings in children with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(3):395–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Gaigg SB, Cornell AS, Bird G. The Psychophysiological mechanisms of alexithymia in autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2018;22(2):227–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Silani G, Bird G, Brindley R, Singer T, Frith C, Frith U. Levels of emotional awareness and autism: an fMRI study. Soc Neurosci. 2008;3(2):97–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Khoury NM, Lutz J, Schuman-Olivier Z. Interoception in psychiatric disorders: a review of randomized, controlled trials with interoception-based interventions. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2018;26(5):250–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Salles BM, De Maturana W, Dos Santos VA, Mograbi DC. Effects of DBT-based interventions on alexithymia: a systematic review. Cogn Behav Ther. 2023;52(2):110–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Ellison WD, Trahan AC, Pinzon JC, Gillespie ME, Simmons LM, King KY. For whom, and for what, is experience sampling more accurate than retrospective report? Pers Individ Dif. 2020;163:110071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Santangelo P, Bohus M, Ebner-Priemer UW. Ecological momentary assessment in borderline personality disorder: a review of recent findings and methodological challenges. J Pers Disord. 2014;28(4):555–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Khor AS, Gray KM, Reid SC, Melvin GA. Feasibility and validity of ecological momentary assessment in adolescents with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. J Adolesc. 2014;37(1):37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Conner TS, Mehl MR. Ambulatory assessment: methods for studying everyday life. In: Scott RA, Kosslyn SM, editors. Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences: An interdisciplinary, searchable, and linkable resource. 1st ed. Wiley; 2015. pp. 1–15.

  70. Ebner-Priemer UW, Kuo J, Kleindienst N, Welch SS, Reisch T, Reinhard I, et al. State affective instability in borderline personality disorder assessed by ambulatory monitoring. Psychol Med. 2007;37(7):961–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Webb CA, Forgeard M, Israel ES, Lovell-Smith N, Beard C, Björgvinsson T. Personalized prescriptions of therapeutic skills from patient characteristics: an ecological momentary assessment approach. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2022;90(1):51–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Stiglmayr CE, Grathwol T, Linehan MM, Ihorst G, Fahrenberg J, Bohus M. Aversive tension in patients with borderline personality disorder: a computer-based controlled field study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;111(5):372–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Solhan MB, Trull TJ, Jahng S, Wood PK. Clinical assessment of affective instability: comparing EMA indices, questionnaire reports, and retrospective recall. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(3):425–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Ebner-Priemer UW, Kuo J, Schlotz W, Kleindienst N, Rosenthal MZ, Detterer L, et al. Distress and affective dysregulation in patients with borderline personality disorder: a psychophysiological ambulatory monitoring study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196(4):314–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Costache ME, Gioia F, Vanello N, Greco A, Lefebvre F, Capobianco A et al. Exploring emotion control and alexithymia in autistic adults: an ecological momentary assessment study. 2024.

  76. Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Hus V, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: revised algorithm and standardized severity scores. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(8):1996–2012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Penttilä J, Kuusela T, Scheinin H. Analysis of rapid heart rate variability in the assessment of anticholinergic drug effects in humans. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2005;61(8):559–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Briet J, Javelot H, Heitzmann E, Weiner L, Lameira C, D’Athis P, et al. The anticholinergic impregnation scale: towards the elaboration of a scale adapted to prescriptions in French psychiatric settings. Therapies. 2017;72(4):427–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Dan-Glauser ES, Scherer KR. The difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS): factor structure and consistency of a French translation. Swiss J Psychol. 2012;72(1):5–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Williams ZJ, Gotham KO. Improving the measurement of alexithymia in autistic adults: a psychometric investigation of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale and generation of a general alexithymia factor score using item response theory. Mol Autism. 2021;12(1):56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Schuurmans AAT, De Looff P, Nijhof KS, Rosada C, Scholte RHJ, Popma A, et al. Validity of the empatica E4 wristband to measure heart rate variability (HRV) parameters: a comparison to electrocardiography (ECG). J Med Syst. 2020;44(11):190.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. The jamovi project. Jamovi (Version 2.3) [Computer Software]. 2023. Available from: https://www.jamovi.org.

  84. Quené H, Van Den Bergh H. On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: a tutorial. Speech Commun. 2004;43(1–2):103–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  86. Nezlek JB. Multilevel modeling for psychologists. In: Cooper H, Camic PM, Long DL, Panter AT, Rindskopf D, Sher KJ, editors. APA handbook of research methods in psychology. Data analysis and research publication. Volume 3. Washington: American Psychological Association; 2012. pp. 219–41.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Linehan MM. Dialectical behavior therapy in clinical practice. New York: Guilford; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Rieffe C, Meerum Terwogt M, Kotronopoulou K. Awareness of single and multiple emotions in high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(3):455–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. What’s in the name ‘alexithymia’? A commentary on affective agnosia: expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:1006–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Frazier SN, Vela J. Dialectical behavior therapy for the treatment of anger and aggressive behavior: a review. Aggress Violent Behav. 2014;19(2):156–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Chen SY, Cheng Y, Zhao WW, Zhang YH. Effects of dialectical behavior therapy on reducing self-harming behaviors and negative emotions in patients with borderline personality disorder: a meta‐analysis. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2021;28(6):1128–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Kraiss JT, Ten Klooster PM, Moskowitz JT, Bohlmeijer ET. The relationship between emotion regulation and well-being in patients with mental disorders: a meta-analysis. Compr Psychiatry. 2020;102:152189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26:41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Kleiman EM, Rizvi SL, Duberstein PR. Initial validation of an ecological momentary assessment measure of self-efficacy to manage negative emotions. Assessment. 2023;30(7):2223–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Riviello MT, Capuano V, Ombrato G, Baldassarre I, Cordasco G, Esposito A. The influence of positive and negative emotions on physiological responses and memory task scores. In: Bassis S, Esposito A, Morabito FC, editors. Recent advances of neural network models and applications. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2014. p. 315–23. (Smart Innovation, Systems and Technologies; vol. 26). Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-3-319-04129-2_31. [cited 2024 Dec 12].

  96. Pinna T, Edwards DJ. A systematic review of associations between interoception, vagal tone, and emotional regulation: potential applications for mental health, wellbeing, psychological flexibility, and chronic conditions. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1792.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Allen B, Jennings JR, Gianaros PJ, Thayer JF, Manuck SB. Resting high-frequency heart rate variability is related to resting brain perfusion. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(2):277–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Tuck NL, Grant RCI, Sollers JJ, Booth RJ, Consedine NS. Higher resting heart rate variability predicts skill in expressing some emotions. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(12):1852–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. McCarthy C, Pradhan N, Redpath C, Adler A. Validation of the Empatica E4 wristband. In: 2016 IEEE EMBS International Student Conference (ISC). Ottawa: IEEE; 2016. pp. 1–4. Available from: http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/7508621/. [cited 2024 Apr 17].

  100. Neacsiu AD, Eberle JW, Kramer R, Wiesmann T, Linehan MM. Dialectical behavior therapy skills for transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2014;59:40–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Mazurek MO, Lu F, Macklin EA, Handen BL. Factors associated with DSM-5 severity level ratings for autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2019;23(2):468–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We extend our gratitude to the participants and health professionals whose collaboration was vital to this research. In particular, we would like to thank Doha Bemmouna and Thomas Paulet for their clinical dedication and invaluable contribution to both group and individual therapy.

Funding

The project was financially supported by the John Bost Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MEC: conceptualisation, methodology, project management, data collection, data curation, software, data analysis, writing – original draft, writing – review and editing; FG: software, data curation, data analysis; NV, AG: data curation, supervision; AC: conceptualisation, methodology, funding acquisition; SW & LW: conceptualisation, methodology, funding acquisition, data curation, supervision, writing – review and editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mădălina Elena Costache.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The authors confirm that all procedures undertaken in the course of this research project comply with the ethical standards set out by the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The Regional Ethics Committee of Eastern France approved this study as a preliminary step towards a randomised controlled trial of dialectical behavior therapy and emotion dysregulation (reference number: SI 21.01.21.41923). The study protocol has been registered on the clinicaltrials.gov (registration number: NCT04737707). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their involvement in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Costache, M.E., Gioia, F., Vanello, N. et al. Dialectical behavior therapy in autistic adults: effects on ecological subjective and physiological measures of emotion dysregulation. bord personal disord emot dysregul 12, 14 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40479-025-00288-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40479-025-00288-1

Keywords